Discuss some of the cultural influences on mathematics performance as delineated in the text. Why is it important to have an understanding of cultural differences in math ability?

What will be an ideal response?

American schoolchildren perform significantly more poorly in mathematics than children from East Asian cultures, beginning in the first grade, with the magnitude of the cultural difference increasing with age. In attempting to explain these findings, researchers quickly ruled out the possibility that East Asian students are inherently smarter than Americans; first-graders in the US, Taiwan, and Japan perform equally well on standardized intelligence tests (Stevenson et al., 1985). Yet East Asian first-graders already rely on a more sophisticated mix of basic arithmetic strategies than American first-graders do, including the relatively sophisticated (for first-graders) decomposition and fact-retrieval strategies. And other research reveals that the math-strategy advantage that East Asian children display is already apparent during the preschool period (Geary et al., 1993).

Basic differences in how the Chinese (and Japanese and Korean) versus the English language represents numbers seems to contribute to some of the early differences in arithmetic proficiency. Recall from Chapter 7 that the number words in Chinese for 11, 12, and 13 are translated as "ten-one," "ten-two," and "ten-three," which helps children learn to count sooner than American children, who must use the more idiosyncratic number words of "11," "12," and "13.". The Chinese number-naming system also helps children to understand that the one in 13 has a place value of 10 (rather than one). By contrast, English words for two-digit numbers in the teens are irregular and do not convey the idea of tens and ones.

Several East Asian instructional practices support the rapid learning of math facts and computational procedures involved in multidigit addition and subtraction. East Asian students practice computational procedures more than American students do, and practice of this sort fosters the retrieval of math facts from memory. The type of instruction provided also seems to matter. For example, Asian teachers instructing students how to carry a sum from one column of a multidigit number to the next will say to "bring up" the sum instead of "carrying" it. The term bring up (rather than carry) may help children learning multidigit addition to remember that each digit to the left in a multidigit number is a base-10 increment of the cardinal value of that digit.

Differences in mathematical competencies between East Asian and American students seem to be a relatively recent phenomenon that undoubtedly reflects broader cultural differences in educational philosophies and supports for education and the differences in linguistic and instructional supports for mathematics learning that we have discussed here.

Psychology

You might also like to view...

Which of the following is an experimental variable?

a) an individual discussion of life history of depression b) an equal probability of being assigned to either the group receiving treatment for claustrophobia or to the group not receiving the treatment c) a relationship between nyctophobia and claustrophobia d) a treatment for claustrophobia

Psychology

Researchers studying spatial mental models have asked participants to develop mental representations of settings based on text material. When the participants then are asked to use these mental models to answer questions about objects in the setting,

a. performance is affected by the order in which the objects are presented. b. where the objects are located in the participants' mental models affects their speed of access to the information. c. participants are faster to locate objects that are not in view in their mental models. d. few participants are able to respond correctly, suggesting that only some participants form mental models.

Psychology